Is dominance a useful concept in dog behavior? In the past, much of our thoughts about dominance in dogs came from captive wolf populations where there was a linear dominance hierarchy, with an alpha wolf who was most dominant, then a beta wolf who was dominant over everyone except the alpha, and so on down the line, until we got to the omega wolf who was dominant over no one.
When wild wolves were studied, the concept of dominance in wolves became much looser, because wild wolf social groups are either family units or extended family units (parents, young, and siblings of parents), and adult wolves tend to assume more dominant positions over the younger wolves, but otherwise the situation is fairly fluid. (See the March 19, 2010 post on this blog by Dr. Suzanne Hetts on dominance myths, and the YouTube video by Dr. L. David Mech on wolf dominance at: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=tNtFgdwTsbU).
But our training methods of dogs go back to the concepts of dominance derived from captive wolf groups.
Some of our training methods assume that we have to be physically dominant over our dogs. This involves in extreme cases the alpha roll, or rolling the dog over on his back and standing over him (something that I personally do not recommend, as it is a way to get bitten and possibly traumatize everyone concerned, including the dog). Other methods involve pushing the dog down for a "sit" or a "down" command, and yanking a dog around on a leash.
Non-physical methods include giving dogs less priority of access to resources such as food or space. For example, a dog might be required to wait before getting food, or might be expected to tolerate having her food bowl taken away by her people half-way through her dinner without her showing any signs of aggression.
But dogs are not wolves, even though genetically they share more than 99 percent of their genes with wolves.
And because dogs have been domesticated for a long time, it is hard to get humans out of the equation and find out what is normal for social groups of dogs when they are not interacting with people.
Surprisingly few studies have looked at the social interactions within dog packs that have not been associated with people.
A recent study looked at dominance interactions in a free-ranging social group of dogs that had relatively little contact with humans in the suburbs of Rome, Italy (Cafazzo, S. et al. 2010. Dominance in relation to age, sex, and competitive contexts in a group of domestic dogs. Behavioral Ecology 21: 443-455).
The dog social group had 27-40 dogs, and was observed for a total of 15 months. During that time, the composition of the group changed by some dogs leaving, others joining the social group, some dogs dying, and some pups being born to females. The authors of the study present their results based on observations of 6 adult males, 5 adult females, 1 subadult female, 4 subadult males, 5 juvenile females and 6 juvenile males, or 27 dogs in all.
The authors characterized dominance interactions as two kinds: agonistic dominance, and formal dominance. They defined agonistic dominance as situations where one dog acts aggressively and the other dog acts submissively. They defined formal dominance as situations where one dog gives another dog a submissive signal without the first dog showing any aggression. They studied dominance interactions under the following conditions: 1) when there were no resources for the dogs to compete; 2) when the dogs had to compete over food; 2) when the male dogs had to compete over receptive females.
The bottom line in this study is that dominant status did not always correlate well with aggression. The aggressive dogs were not necessarily the most dominant in terms of submission of other dogs when there was no competition for food or females.
Dominant status did correlate well with the formal dominance described by the authors, where one dog produced signals that another dog responded to with submission. Such formal dominance gave dogs priority of access to food when they were competing, and gave some males priority of access to receptive females.
So what does this study mean for us?
One take-home message is that you don't need to be aggressive with your dog for her to acknowledge that you are more dominant (or at least, for you to have higher status than your dog). All you need to do is have the right kind of signals that your dog understands. In other words you have to learn to communicate with your dog rather than beat her into submission.
Another take-home message is that despite our long association with dogs, we still don't really know how they think. We know what dominance means to us, but we are still struggling to find out what it means to dogs, and whether our idea of dominance is even a meaningful concept for our furry friends.
--Con Slobodchikoff
Recent Comments